Thirst

views updated May 08 2018

THIRST

THIRST. Thirst is a conscious sensation that results in a desire to drink. Although all normal humans experience thirst, science can offer no precise definition of this phenomenon because it involves numerous physiological responses to a change in internal fluid status, complex patterns of central nervous system function, and psychological motivation. Three factors are typically recognized as components of thirst: a body water deficit, brain integration of central and peripheral nerve messages relating to the need for water, and an urge to drink. In laboratory experiments, thirst is measured empirically with subjective perceptual scales (for example, ranging from "not thirsty at all" to "very, very thirsty") and drinking behavior is quantified by observing the timing and volume of fluid consumed.

Psychologists classify thirst as a drive, a basic compelling urge that motivates action. Other human drives involve a lack of nutrients (for example, glucose, sodium), oxygen, or sleep; these are satiated by eating, breathing, and sleeping. Clark Hull published a major, relevant theory describing the nature of human drives in 1943. He observed that learned habits, in addition to the thirst drive, influence drinking strongly. If a behavior reduces thirst, that behavior is reinforced and learned as a habit. Irrelevant behaviors (for example, sneezing, grooming) provide no reinforcement, have no effect on drinking, and do not become habits. Further, Hull realized that external incentives, such as the qualities or quantity of a fluid, also influence fluid consumption. On a hot summer day, for example, a cold beverage is more attractive than a cup of hot tea. Yet when chilled to a very low temperature, a cold beverage becomes an aversive stimulus to drinking behavior. Physiologists have popularized the term alliesthesia (from Greek root words referring to altered sensation) to describe the fact that the sensation of thirst may have either pleasant or unpleasant qualities, depending on the intensity of the stimulus and the state of the person.

Numerous investigations have verified that thirst and drinking behavior are complex entities. For example, drinking behavior (that is, the timing and the amount of fluid consumed) is not linearly related to the intensity of perceived thirst. Nor should we infer that individuals experience thirst simply because they drink. These facts indicate that thirst and drinking behavior are distinct entities that influence each other and are influenced by numerous internal and external factors.

Physiological Components of Thirst

Thirst is often viewed by physiologists and physicians as a central nervous system mechanism that regulates the body's water and minerals. The significance of the thirst drive is emphasized by three facts: 50 to 70 percent of adult body weight is water, the average adult ingests and loses 2.5 liters of water each day, and body weight is regulated within 0.2 percent from one day to the next. Clearly, water is essential to life and the body responds in a manner that ensures survival.

In 1954, Edward Adolph and colleagues proposed a multiple-factor theory of thirst that has not been refuted to date. This theory states that no single mechanism can account for all drinking behavior and that multiple mechanisms, sometimes with identical functions, act concurrently. Because water is essential to life, the existence of redundant mechanisms has great survival value. Among these, thirst appears to be regulated primarily by evaluation of changes in the concentration of extracellular fluid, measured as the osmolality of blood plasma. (Osmolality is a measurement that describes the concentration of all dissolved solids in a solution, that is, dissolved substances per unit of solvent. In research and clinical laboratories, the unit for osmolality of blood is mOsm/kg or milliosmoles per kilogram of water.)

Below a certain threshold level of plasma osmolality, thirst is absent. Above this threshold, a strong desire to drink appears in response to an increase of 2 to 3 percent in the level of dissolved substances in blood. The brain's thirst center lies deep within the brain, in an area known as the hypothalamus. This anatomical site contains cells that respond to changes in the concentration of body fluids. When the thirst center is stimulated by an increased concentration of blood (that is, dehydration), thirst and fluid consumption increase.

As the brain senses the concentration of blood, it allows a minor loss of body water before stimulating the drive to drink. This phenomenon has been named voluntary dehydration. Specifically, several research studies since the 1930s have observed that adults and children replace only 34 to 87 percent of the water lost as sweat, by drinking during exercise or labor in hot environments. The resulting dehydration is due to the fact that thirst is not perceived until a 1 to 2 percent body weight loss occurs. Interindividual differences, resulting in great voluntary dehydration in some individuals, have caused them to be named reluctant drinkers.

Reduced extracellular fluid volume, including blood volume, also increases thirst. Experiments (for example, reducing blood volume without altering blood concentration) have demonstrated that volume-sensitive receptors in the heart and blood vessels likely regulate drinking behavior by increasing the secretion of hormones. This effect is relatively minor, however. Animal research suggests that a change in extracellular fluid concentration accounts for most (for example, 70 percent) of the increased fluid consumption that follows moderate whole-body dehydration, whereas a decrease of fluid volume per se plays a secondary role.

Thus, thirst is extinguished when body fluid concentration decreases and fluid volume increases. Osmolality-sensitive nerves in the mouth, throat, and stomach also play a role in abating thirst. As fluid passes through the mouth and upper gastrointestinal tract, the sense of dryness decreases. When this fluid fills the stomach, stretch receptors sense an increase in gastric fullness and the thirst drive diminishes.

As dehydration causes the body's extracellular fluid to become more concentrated, the fluid inside cells moves outward, resulting in intracellular dehydration and cell shrinkage, and the hormone arginine vasopressin (AVP, also known as the antidiuretic hormone) is released from the brain. AVP serves two purposes: to reduce urine output at the kidneys and to enhance thirst; both serve to restore normal fluid balance. Other hormones influence fluid-mineral balance directly and thirst indirectly. Renin, angiotensin II, and aldosterone are noteworthy examples. As dehydration reduces circulating blood volume, blood pressure decreases and renin is secreted from blood vessels inside the kidneys. Renin activates the hormone angiotensin II, which subsequently stimulates the release of aldosterone from the adrenal glands. Both angiotensin II and aldosterone increase blood pressure and enhance the retention of sodium and water; these effects indirectly reduce the intensity of thirst. Angiotensin II also affects thirst directly. When injected into sensitive areas of the brain, it causes a rapid increase in water consumption that is followed by a slower increase in sodium chloride consumption and water retention by the kidneys.

Host Factors

Repeated training sessions in cool or hot environments alter fluid consumption in four ways. First, physical training increases the secretion of the hormone AVP, which stimulates drinking and body water retention. Second, exercise-heat acclimation (that is, adaptations due to exercise in a hot environment over eight days) increases the volume of fluid consumed and the number of times that adults drink during exercise. Third, frequent rest periods, in the midst of labor or exercise, will increase fluid replacement time and enhance fluid consumption. Humans tend to drink less when they are preoccupied or are performing physical or mental tasks. Fourth, learned behaviors can enhance fluid consumption when thirst is absent. This phenomenon is widely appreciated among military personnel and athletes who are trained to consume water at regular intervals, whether they are thirsty or not.

Several research groups have reported that chronological age influences thirst and drinking behavior. Elderly men experience a blunted thirst drive and reduced fluid intake, perhaps due to their brains' reduced ability to sense changes in plasma osmolality or blood volume. Further, elderly individuals experience a decrease in the ability of their kidneys to conserve water. This suggests that the elderly are predisposed to dehydration when illness increases water loss (that is, vomiting, diarrhea) or when physical incapacity prevents access to water.

Fluid and Environmental Characteristics

Many fluid characteristics stimulate or enhance drinking, during or after exposure to a hot environment. Fluid temperature (consumption is greatest at 14 to 16°C, reduced above 37°C), turbidity, sweetness, fruit flavorings (for example, cherry, grape, orange, lemon), addition of citric acid which imparts a citrus flavor, and addition of sodium chloride or other minerals are examples. These components enhance palatability and increase fluid consumption. The addition of a small amount of salt (sodium chloride), besides enhancing palatability, may result in thirst and increased drinking, due to the specific action of sodium on fluid movements. An increased sodium concentration outside of cells causes water to leave cells via osmosis. The resulting cellular dehydration is an important stimulus for drinking. Increased beverage carbonation tends to reduce the palatability of a fluid as well as the volume of fluid consumed, without an increase in thirst. In addition, intakes of food and water are closely related. During 24-hour observations of fluid intake, most studies report that the majority of fluid (69 to 78 percent) is consumed during meals. The foregoing characteristics, therefore, tend to reduce the magnitude of voluntary dehydration.

Conversely, fluid characteristics may influence drinking behavior negatively, regardless of the intensity of thirst. Experiments conducted during mild prolonged exercise have shown that the following qualities are perceived as undesirable: nausea, bloating, an objectionable feeling in the mouth, excessive viscosity, and excessive sweetness (see Passe, 1996). Exercise and high ambient temperature may independently alter an individual's perception of fluid palatability. For example, drinking behavior increases when air temperature exceeds 25°C. Fluid consumption can also be enhanced by changing the shape of a fluid container, proximity of fluid containers to the drinker, volume of fluid that is available, and time allowed for drinking.

Societal customs may influence fluid consumption, as evidenced by cross-cultural differences in beverage preferences. Even rituals, such as accepting the friendly offer of a beverage in a social setting, may enhance fluid intake beyond that driven by physiological cues. These factors usually involve learned habits. Similarly, when people repeatedly drink fluids with initially unfamiliar flavors, the palatability of the fluids is enhanced.

Although a comprehensive theory of thirst and fluid balance eludes description, it is likely that the thirst drive increases and diminishes because multiple factors (for example, oral dryness, gastric distension, osmolality, volume, fluid qualities) are integrated concurrently by the brain's thirst center.

See also Alcohol ; Appetite ; Beer ; Cocktails ; Coffee ; Grapes and Grape Juice ; Sensation and the Senses ; Tea ; Water ; Wine .

BIBLIOGRAPHY

Adolph, Edward F., June P. Barker, and Patricia A. Hoy. "Multiple Factors in Thirst." American Journal of Physiology 178 (1954): 538562.

Armstrong, Lawrence E., Roger W. Hubbard, Patricia C. Szlyk, William T. Matthew, and Ingrid V. Sils. "Voluntary Dehydration and Electrolyte Losses During Prolonged Exercise in the Heat." Aviation, Space and Environmental Medicine 56 (1985): 765770.

Armstrong, Lawrence E., and Carl M. Maresh. "Fluid Replacement During Exercise and Recovery from Exercise." In Body Fluid Balance in Exercise and Sport, edited by Elsworth R. Buskirk and Susan M. Puhl. Boca Raton, Fla.: CRC Press, 1996.

Cabanac, Michel. "Physiological Role of Pleasure." Science 173 (1971): 11031107.

Engell, Diane, and Edward Hirsch. "Environmental and Sensory Modulation of Fluid Intake in Humans." In Thirst: Psychological and Physiological Aspects. edited by David J. Ramsay and David Booth, pp. 382389. London: Springer-Verlag, 1991.

Fitzsimons, J. T. "Thirst and Sodium Appetite in the Regulation of Body Fluids." In Control Mechanisms of Drinking, edited by G. Peters, J. T. Fitzsimons, and L. Peters-Haefeli. New York: Springer-Verlag, 1975.

Fitzsimons, J. T. "Angiotensin, Thirst, and Sodium Appetite." Physiological Reviews 78 (1998): 583675.

Greenleaf, John E. "Problem: Thirst, Drinking Behavior, and Involuntary Dehydration. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise 24 (1992): 645656.

Greenleaf, John E., and Taketoshi Morimoto. "Mechanisms Controlling Fluid Ingestion: Thirst and Drinking." In Body Fluid Balance: Exercise and Sport, edited by Ellsworth R. Buskirk and Susan M. Puhl, pp. 317. Boca Raton, Fla.: CRC Press, 1996.

Hubbard, Roger W., Barbara Sandick, William T. Matthew, Ralph P. Francesconi, James B. Sampson, Michael J. Durkot, Maller Owen, and Diane B. Engell. "Voluntary Dehydration and Alliesthesia for Water." Journal of Applied Physiology: Respiratory, Environmental, Exercise Physiology 57 (1984): 868875.

Hubbard, Roger W., Patricia C. Szlyk, and Lawrence E. Armstrong. "Influence of Thirst and Fluid Palatability on Fluid Ingestion During Exercise." In Perspectives in Exercise Sciences and Sports Medicine: Fluid Homeostasis During Exercise, pp. 39-96. Indianapolis, Ind.: Benchmark Press, 1990.

Hull, Clark. "Primary Motivation and Reaction Potential." In Principles of Behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1943.

Passe, Dennis H. "Physiological and Psychological Determinants of Fluid Intake." In Sport Drinks: Basic Science and Practical Aspects, edited by Ronald J. Maughan and Robert Murray. Boca Raton, Fla.: CRC Press, 1996.

Lawrence E. Armstrong


Factors That Alter Thirst

Increase Thirst

  • increased concentration of blood
  • decreased blood volume
  • decreased blood pressure
  • mouth and throat dryness
  • increased angiotensin II

Decrease Thirst

  • decreased concentration of blood
  • increased blood volume
  • increased blood pressure
  • increased stomach fullness
  • decreased angiotensin II

thirst

views updated Jun 11 2018

thirst / [unvoicedth]ərst/ • n. a feeling of needing or wanting to drink something: they quenched their thirst with spring water. ∎  lack of the liquid needed to sustain life: tens of thousands died of thirst and starvation. ∎  (usu. thirst for) poetic/lit. a strong desire for something: his thirst for knowledge was mainly academic.• v. [intr.] archaic (of a person or animal) feel a need to drink something. ∎  (usu. thirst for/after) poetic/lit. have a strong desire for something: an opponent thirsting for revenge.

thirst

views updated May 29 2018

thirst sb. OE. þurst = OS thurst (Du. dorst), (O)HG. durst :- WGmc. *þurstu (cf. ON. þorsti, Goth. þaurstei); f. *þurs- :- IE. *tṛs- *tors-, repr. also by L. torrēre dry, parch, Skr. tṛ́ṣyati thirsts, OIr. tart.
So thirst vb. OE. þyrstan (till late ME. also impers.).

Thirst

views updated Jun 27 2018

Thirst ★★ 1987 (R)

A girl is abducted by a secret society that wants her to become their new leader. There is just one catch: she has to learn to like the taste of human blood. Chilling but weakly plotted. 96m/C VHS, DVD . David Hemmings, Henry Silva, Chantal Contouri; D: Rod Hardy.

thirst

views updated May 29 2018

thirst See water balance.

Thirst

views updated Jun 11 2018

641. Thirst

  1. Ancient Mariner he and his crew nearly die of thirst. [Br. Poetry: Coleridge The Ancient Mariner ]

More From encyclopedia.com